Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

by Hidden Brain, Shankar Vedantam

Trending Podcast Topics, In Your Inbox

Sign up for Beacon’s free newsletter, and find out about the most interesting podcast topics before everyone else.

Rated 5 stars by early readers

By continuing, you are indicating that you accept our Terms of Service and Privacy Policy.

Topics in this Episode

About This Episode

52:04 minutes

published 20 days ago

English

All Rights Reserved

Speaker 20s - 16.3s

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam PERSON. Philip Zimbardo grew up poor in New York City in the South Bronx LOC. As he went to school and played in his neighborhood, he noticed something. There were lots of ways for kids from poor families to get into trouble.

Speaker 316.9s - 30.2s

One of the things about growing up poor is you're surrounded by evil, meaning people whose job it is to get good kids to do bad things for money. And even as a little kid, I was always curious about why some kids got seduced and other kids like me were

Speaker 230.2s - 49.84s

able to resist. Was some kids smarter, tougher? Lots of people might draw such conclusions, but from an early age, Phil PERSON found himself interested in another explanation. The context in which a good kid would do something bad. The situation. At school...

Speaker 349.84s - 52s

James Monroe High School, also in the Bronx GPE.

Speaker 252.3s - 55.82s

Phil PERSON got close to a classmate who was interested in the same questions.

Speaker 356.06s - 71.1s

And it was a little Jewish kid named Stanley Milgram PERSON. We were in the same class. We sat side by side. He was the smartest kid in the class. He won all the medals at graduation, so obviously nobody liked him because we were all envious of him. But he was super smart and super serious.

Speaker 274.18s - 140s

If you knew anything about psychology, you will know that these teenagers went on to become two of the most influential psychologists in history. Phil became famous for conducting the Stanford Prison Experiment EVENT, where he turned the university's psychology department into a makeshift prison. Stanley Milgram PERSON made his mark with a study that examined the power of situations to seduce good people to do bad things. It involved asking a volunteer to administer a memory test to another person.If the answers were wrong, the volunteer was told to deliver a series of electrical shocks as punishment. The study has invited a great deal of admiration and a great deal of criticism over the years. We're going to begin today's show by taking you through this famous experiment. As you listen, pay attention to how you're responding to the scene that unfolds, what you think about the different characters and how you relate to them. Once it's done, we're going to talk with a psychologist who realizedthat most people overlook something in the experiment.

Speaker 17140s - 153.96s

We so often sort of simulate, if I was in that Milgram Shock experiment, what would I do if I was the study participant, right? Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no? But we kind of flipped that idea on its head.

Speaker 2158.22s - 294.4s

Today on the show, we continue our Innovation 2.0 series by looking at the underappreciated power we exert on others and how this knowledge can transform our relationships, both at work and in our personal lives. Flipping the script, this week on Hidden Brain comes from Alta Beauty ORG. This AAPI Heritage Month, Alta Beauty ORG is celebrating the joy of belonging, belonging to a community composed of intricate connections,belonging to the heritage and birthright that is beauty. Alta Beauty spotlights the AAPI NORP community, passing the mic to brand founders and creators to tell their stories centered on heritage, joy, and beauty. Shop AAPI owned and founded brands at AltaBeauty Stores and Ulta.com ORG. Support for Hidden Brain comes from FedEx ORG.Small and medium businesses need happy customers. That's why FedEx ORG offers picture-proof of delivery, package-less and paperless returns, and weekend home delivery to 98% of the U.S. GPE on Saturday and 50% on Sunday. See the FedEx ORG Service Guide for delivery information.FedEx Ground Service is also faster to more locations than UPS ORG ground. See what FedEx ORG can do for your business. Absolutely, Positively FedEx ORG. do for your business. Absolutely, positively FedEx. Support for HiddenBrin comes from Progressive ORG, where drivers who save by switching save nearly $750 on average,plus auto customers qualify for an average of seven discounts. Quote now at progressive.com to see if you could save. Progressive Casualty Insurance Company and Affiliates ORG. National average 12-month savings of $744 by new customers surveyed who saved with Progressive between June 2022 and May 20203. Potential savings will vary. Discounts not available in all states and situations.

Speaker 9298.9s - 299.66s

Stanley Milgram PERSON grew up in a world that seemed bent on destroying itself.

Speaker 2304.98s - 305.76s

World War II was raging in Europe and Asia, and by the time he was eight, the U.S. GPE was swept up in the conflict.

Speaker 0306.38s - 313.5s

We interrupt this broadcast to bring to this important bulletin from the United Press ORG. The White House announces Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor FAC.

Speaker 6313.5s - 317.86s

1941, a date which will live in infamate.

Speaker 2317.92s - 322.4s

By tomorrow morning, members of Congress ORG will have a fore-report and be ready for action.

Speaker 0323.4s - 326.2s

The fields of battle were far from Stanley PERSON's home.

Speaker 6326.96s - 330.96s

But as he grew older, he couldn't stop thinking about the war and its implications.

Speaker 0332.18s - 334.3s

Stanley PERSON was consumed by some big questions.

Speaker 2335.24s - 338.16s

Why did so many people willingly kill Jews in the Holocaust EVENT?

Speaker 6339s - 342.1s

Was everyone who followed Nazi NORP orders inherently evil?

Speaker 2342.86s - 344.96s

Here he is in an educational film.

Speaker 5344.96s - 355.54s

How is it possible, I ask myself, that ordinary people were courteous and decent in everyday life can act callously, inhumanely, without any limitations of conscience?

Speaker 2356.46s - 361.6s

Phil Zimbardo PERSON remembers his classmate asking those same questions at James Monroe High School ORG.

Speaker 3361.86s - 381.54s

As a high school student, he was worried that the Holocaust could happen again in America GPE. And everybody said, Stanley, that was Nazi Germany GPE, that was then, we're not that kind of people. And he would say, I'll bet they thought the same thing. And the bottom line, he says,how do you know how you would act unless you're in the situation? How do you know how you would act

Speaker 2381.54s - 412.5s

unless you're in the situation? Stanley PERSON's theory was that the context that people found themselves in shaped their behavior. This went for Nazis NORP, but it went for ordinary people too. Most of us never get to find out if we will behave like Nazis because most of us never find ourselves in situations where we're asked to behave like Nazis NORP. where we are asked to behave like Nazis NORP.By the early 1960s, as a psychology professor at Yale, Stanley PERSON decided to test this idea.

Speaker 4415.26s - 421.34s

Under what conditions would a person obey authority who commanded actions that went against conscience? These are exactly the questions that I wanted to investigate at Yale University ORG.

Speaker 2422.36s - 516.76s

Stanley PERSON wanted to put volunteers in a situation where they would be asked to do something that was clearly wrong. Would they do it? Follow instructions? Obey orders? He came up with a scenario that was simple, ingenious, and wildly controversial. An experimenter wearing a lab coat invited volunteers into a room. The volunteers were told they were part of a study about learning and memory.Some would play the role of teacher, while others would play a student. What you're going to hear next is a recreation of the study using voice actors. The dialogue is drawn from a 1962 documentary that describes the experiment. Before we begin, we should know that some listeners may find this section upsetting because it involves descriptions of someone inflicting pain on another person. Also, there are two liberties we've taken in this recreation. First, in the real version of this experiment,the student responded to the teacher's questions by silently flipping a switch. We've given voice to those actions. Second, we've imagined the internal monologues of some of the people in the experiment. Those inner voices sound different from the things they say aloud, and you'll hear them both throughout the scene. So, that's a setup for the scene. So, that's a setup for the experiment. Remember, there was an experimenter and two volunteers, one playingthe role of teacher and the other playing the role of student. The experimenter began by explaining

Speaker 8516.76s - 527.04s

the purpose of the memory test. We want to find out just what affect different people have on each other as teachers and learners, and also what effect punishment will have on learning in this situation.

Speaker 2527.72s - 545.5s

The experimenter told the person playing the role of student to sit in a chair. The experimenter strapped down the student's arms and attached an electrode to his wrist. The electrode, the student was told, was connected to a shock generator. Then the experimenter explained,

Speaker 8545.82s - 560.82s

The teacher will read a list of word pairs to you like these, blue, girl, nice day, fat neck, and so forth. You are to try to remember each pair. For the next time through, the teacher will read only the first word or the first half of the word pair.

Speaker 2561.44s - 564.64s

The student was asked to remember the second half of the word pair.

Speaker 8565.3s - 567.3s

The experimenter made sure to ask.

Speaker 2567.46s - 568.38s

Do you have any questions now

Speaker 8568.38s - 569.68s

before we go to the next room?

Speaker 10570.64s - 585.24s

No, but I think I should say this. About two years ago, I was at the Veterans Hospital in West Haven GPE, and while there, they detected a heart condition. There's nothing serious, but as long as I'm having these shocks,how strong are they? How dangerous are they?

Speaker 8585.32s - 588.2s

Well, no. Although they may be painful, they're not dangerous.

Speaker 2589.58s - 596.74s

Next, the experimenter ushered the volunteer playing the role of teacher into another room. He gave him a set of instructions.

Speaker 8596.74s - 627.26s

You will read each pair of words in this list once to the learner until you've read the entire list. Direct your voice toward the microphone as the rooms are only partly soundproof. Now, if he gives the correct answer, you say correct and go on to the next line. The correct answer is underline. So it is indicated in the right margin.I see. All right. Looks easy enough. The experiment got underway. Attention learner, your teacher is about to begin the test. Try and remember the word pairs. Ready? Begin.Blue, girl.

Speaker 2627.38s - 628.06s

Right so far.

Speaker 10628.12s - 630.02s

Nice. I think day.

Speaker 2630.1s - 644s

Fat. But when the person in the other room made a mistake... Fat. Was it hat? No. Wet.The volunteer playing the role of teacher would tell the learner that he was wrong. As punishment, he would administer an electric jolt.

Speaker 7644.5s - 650.88s

Incorrect. You'll now get a shock of 75 volts. Oh. Seems nervous.

Speaker 2651.7s - 656.66s

The experimenter in the lab coat, meanwhile, was observing the process. Please continue.

Speaker 7657.28s - 657.62s

Cool.

Speaker 8657.88s - 659.46s

Okay. I'm pretty sure it's day.

Speaker 7659.62s - 662.26s

Wrong. It's head. 105 volts.

Speaker 10662.52s - 662.78s

Ow!

Speaker 2663.3s - 665.44s

Come on. Get it right. I don't want to shock you.

Speaker 7665.56s - 666.72s

Teacher, please continue.

Speaker 8667.32s - 672.38s

What should I do? Is he going to keep going? Boat. I wonder how far I'd go. Wrong. 135 volts, answer.

Speaker 2672.5s - 675.94s

The students trapped to the chair in the other room kept making mistakes.

Speaker 7676.62s - 681.64s

Each time, the experimenter urged the volunteer playing the role of teacher to keep going,

Speaker 2682.18s - 685.26s

to administer a stronger jolt of electricity.

Speaker 7685.38s - 688.04s

Wrong. It's harsh. 150 volts.

Speaker 10688.32s - 691.08s

Ow! Experimenter, get me out of here. That's all. He wants to quit.

Speaker 7691.2s - 695.42s

Get me out of here. I told you I had heart shopper. My heart is starting to bother me now.

Speaker 10695.98s - 700.98s

Get me out of here, please. My heart is starting to bother me. I refuse to go on. Let me out.

Speaker 7700.98s - 703.18s

He refused to go on. You want me to keep going?

Speaker 10703.34s - 709.4s

The experiment requires you to continue, teacher. Please continue. The next word is sad.

Speaker 7709.4s - 712.86s

You're going to stop. Hi, hello? Next word sad.

Speaker 8713.24s - 715.74s

I don't know. Day?

Speaker 7715.92s - 718.62s

Wrong. I'm up to 180 volts.

Speaker 8718.66s - 721.54s

That's right. 180. Are you going to stop? Please continue.

Speaker 7721.56s - 727.14s

As the shocks increased, so did the pain. And so did the protests coming from the next room.

Speaker 10727.26s - 728.68s

He's got a heart condition in there.

Speaker 7729.32s - 730.18s

I'm going to go.

Speaker 10730.38s - 730.98s

Your choice.

Speaker 7731.14s - 731.92s

Please continue.

Speaker 8732.32s - 733.66s

Now I got a shock.

Speaker 7734.58s - 735.38s

180 volts.

Speaker 2735.38s - 735.78s

Ow!

Speaker 7736.28s - 737.66s

I can't stand it.

Speaker 10737.78s - 739.24s

I'm not going to kill a man.

Speaker 7739.24s - 740.82s

So are you going to keep shocking him?

Speaker 8741.26s - 748.78s

Poor guy. He's in there screaming. I said before the shocks may be painful, but they're not dangerous. But he's in there hollering.

Speaker 7748.94s - 752.06s

He can't stand in. What if something happens to happen? You don't have to keep going.

Speaker 2752.94s - 767.16s

The volunteer being asked to administer electric shocks is in a difficult position. The experimenter is urging him to continue, even as the person in the next room begs to be spared. Should he keep going or stop?

Speaker 8767.52s - 770.4s

The experiment requires that you continue, teacher.

Speaker 2770.86s - 775.78s

Whether the learner likes it or not, we must go on until he's learned all the word pairs.

Speaker 8776.06s - 778.54s

I refuse to take responsibility of him getting hurt.

Speaker 2778.66s - 779.96s

So you don't want the responsibility.

Speaker 8780.68s - 783.62s

It's absolutely essential that you continue, teacher.

Speaker 7783.62s - 785.02s

There is too many of them left.

Speaker 8785.14s - 788.66s

I mean, who's going to take the responsibility if anything happens to the gentleman?

Speaker 7788.66s - 790.44s

I'm responsible for anything that happens here.

Speaker 8790.94s - 791.82s

Please continue.

Speaker 7791.82s - 794.64s

As the experiment progressed, the memory test became more demanding.

Speaker 8796.56s - 799.78s

Slow, walk, dance, truck, music.

Speaker 7800.18s - 800.86s

Answer, please.

Speaker 8800.86s - 801.78s

Come on. Get this right.

Speaker 2802.14s - 804.56s

I know it's for science, but I don't want to hurt you.

Speaker 8804.84s - 805.18s

Wrong. 1095 bucks. Oh, let me out of here. Continue, please. Come on. Get this right. I know it's for science, but I don't want to hurt you. Wrong.

Speaker 7805.84s - 806.88s

1095 bucks.

Speaker 10807.18s - 808.1s

Let me out of here.

Speaker 7808.1s - 811.88s

Continue, please. I don't know. Well, the experiment requires.

Speaker 8811.88s - 815.32s

I know it does, sir, but I mean, you do know what he's getting in for.

Speaker 10815.4s - 817.5s

He's up to a hundred and ninety-five volts.

Speaker 8817.5s - 818.04s

Yeah, one hundred ninety-five.

Speaker 2818.14s - 819.1s

That's pretty high.

Speaker 8820.68s - 826.62s

After the study reached about 330 volts, the screams from the next room went silent.

Speaker 2829.2s - 836.06s

If the learner doesn't answer in a reasonable time, about four or five seconds, consider the answer wrong,

Speaker 8836.06s - 839.96s

and follow the same procedures you had been doing for wrong answers.

Speaker 2840.38s - 844.4s

Say, wrong, tell them the number of volts, give him the punishment.

Speaker 8845.04s - 845.5s

Go on, please, with the punishment. Oh, man.

Speaker 2846.28s - 848.52s

Go on, please, with the experiment. Please continue.

Speaker 8848.92s - 850.32s

Is he breaking?

Speaker 2850.8s - 854.9s

Soft, rug, pillow, hair, grass.

Speaker 8855.64s - 856.32s

Answer, please.

Speaker 7858.16s - 859.46s

Go on, teacher.

Speaker 8859.66s - 860.88s

360 volts.

Speaker 7862.64s - 864.72s

I think something's happened to that fellow in there.

Speaker 8864.86s - 866.12s

I didn't get no answer.

Speaker 7866.32s - 870.14s

He was hollering at less voltage. Can you check to see if he's all right, please?

Speaker 8870.26s - 873.34s

Not once we've started. Please continue, teacher.

Speaker 2874.46s - 903.5s

In all, Stanley Milgram ran about 20 different iterations of this study over a span of several years. In this version, many of the volunteers playing the role of teacher showed discomfort, but continued with the experiment. More than half went all the way to 450 volts, even when the screams from the next room went silent, and the student was presumably unconscious.Why didn't the volunteer stop? Stanley PERSON later debriefed some of the volunteers.

Speaker 6903.5s - 913.46s

Why didn't you stop anyway? I did stop, but he kept going and keep going. But why didn't you just disregard what he said? He says it's got to go on, the experiment.

Speaker 2914.56s - 1008.9s

If you're familiar with the study, you already know that the student in the other room was an actor and not actually given electric shocks. The screams and cries of protest were carefully timed recordings. The only target of the experiment were the volunteers who played the role of teacher, the people who had to administer the shocks. Stanley Milgram's PERSON study generated enormous attention and controversy. Admirers drew parallels between the experiment and what happened in Nazi Germany GPE.They said, look, people are sheep. They can be easily misled by demagogues and dictators. Critics of the study said, no, those conclusions are vastly exaggerated. They questioned whether the volunteers actually behaved the way the experiment suggested. Some critics said that many volunteers simply refuse to go along. Beyond the academic debates, the study prompted an entire sub-genre of books and movies.Even today, people find the study fascinating, and they find it fascinating for one reason. How, they ask, could people who know that something is wrong go along with it? Are such people typical? Is everyone susceptible to such influence? Am I? As we listen to the details of the study, we can't help but ask, what would I do?Would I follow orders and zap the person screaming in the other room?

Speaker 71008.96s - 1010.36s

105 volts. Ow!

Speaker 51010.92s - 1011.42s

Come on.

Speaker 71011.66s - 1012.8s

Can't write. I don't want a shock you.

Speaker 81012.8s - 1013.98s

Teacher, please continue.

Speaker 71014.1s - 1014.72s

What should I do?

Speaker 81016.26s - 1022.82s

But Vanessa Bonds, a psychologist at Cornell University ORG, realized there was something no one was paying attention to.

Speaker 71023.7s - 1026.7s

Everyone was asking what was going on in the minds of the volunteers

Speaker 81026.7s - 1029.54s

and how difficult the situation was for them.

Speaker 71029.92s - 1031.66s

We so often sort of simulate,

Speaker 81031.78s - 1033.64s

if I was in that Milgram PERSON shock experiment,

Speaker 71033.84s - 1036.98s

what would I do if I was the study participant, right?

Speaker 81037s - 1040.58s

Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no?

Speaker 71041.36s - 1044.82s

No one was asking whether it was difficult for the experimenter

Speaker 21044.82s - 1045.88s

wearing the lab coat

Speaker 171045.88s - 1049.28s

to tell the volunteers to administer electric shocks.

Speaker 21049.96s - 1061.82s

To the extent we think of the experimenter at all, we might imagine someone who enjoyed putting people in difficult situations, a sort of mad scientist. Vanessa PERSON asked a deceptively simple question.

Speaker 171061.82s - 1067.12s

Was he surprised to see these people going along with this crazy request he was making of them?

Speaker 21067.84s - 1078.34s

Vanessa PERSON's insight was radical. What if you looked at the experiment, not from the point of view of the students screaming in the next room, and not from the point of view of the volunteer administering the shocks,

Speaker 91078.8s - 1081.82s

but from the point of view of the person giving the instructions?

Speaker 21082.62s - 1084.06s

Teacher, please continue.

Speaker 91086.08s - 1086.32s

What if you treated the experiment? I wonder how far ago. What if you giving the instructions? Teacher, please continue. Is he going to keep going? Vote.

Speaker 21086.62s - 1087.58s

I wonder how far I'd go.

Speaker 91088.32s - 1090.36s

What if you treated the experimenter

Speaker 71090.36s - 1092s

as the object of study?

Speaker 81092s - 1092.7s

He wants to quit.

Speaker 71092.82s - 1093.64s

Get me out of here.

Speaker 101093.92s - 1095.24s

I told you I had heart trouble.

Speaker 21095.78s - 1097.62s

Continue, teacher, please go on.

Speaker 101097.62s - 1102.04s

The experiment requires you to continue,

Speaker 81102.34s - 1102.52s

teacher.

Speaker 21102.98s - 1103.82s

Please continue.

Speaker 81104.48s - 1105.6s

Please continue. Please continue.

Speaker 71106.32s - 1110.62s

I said before the shocks may be painful that they're not dangerous.

Speaker 21110.62s - 1112.86s

So are you going to keep shocking him?

Speaker 101114.18s - 1115.32s

So stop.

Speaker 81115.74s - 1117.32s

You don't have to keep going.

Speaker 21117.32s - 1118.28s

You don't have to keep going.

Speaker 81121.52s - 1125.18s

Why do so few of us put ourselves in the shoes of the experimenter?

Speaker 21125.72s - 1129.94s

Why don't we ask how difficult it was for him to issue those instructions?

Speaker 81130.8s - 1136.42s

Why is it, when we hear the story, we automatically put ourselves in the shoes of the volunteers,

Speaker 21136.94s - 1138.66s

the people receiving the instructions?

Speaker 81140.1s - 1144.56s

Vanessa PERSON realized that we all naturally gravitate to the point of view of the volunteers

Speaker 101144.56s - 1147.38s

and not the point of view of the experimenter

Speaker 01147.38s - 1150.8s

because we all instinctively know what it feels like

Speaker 81150.8s - 1153.44s

to have other people put us in uncomfortable situations.

Speaker 21154.44s - 1180.5s

We think of our bosses, our partners, our co-workers, and how they affect our lives and change our moods. We think of the aggressive driver next to us or the other patrons at the restaurant who are so loud and obnoxious that they ruin our meal.We feel buffeted and pushed and pulled by those around us. The one thing we don't ask, what effect do I have on other people?

Speaker 171181.24s - 1196.94s

There's been a long history of research on social influence and persuasion, so we do know a lot about how other people influence us, but we don't know so much about how we experience our influence of other people. When we are intensely focused on how the

Speaker 21196.94s - 1314.28s

world affects us and not how we affect the world, this can have profound consequences for both good and evil. You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam PERSON. Support for Hidden Brain comes from T-Mobile ORG. The most innovative companies are going further with T-Mobile for business. The PGA of America is helping lower scores and elevate fan experiences with AI coaching tools and 5G-connected cameras.AAA ORG is getting more drivers back on the road fast with location telematics, and the Las Vegas Grand Prix is powering race day operations with 5G connectivity, giving fans and experience at the speed they deserve. This is accelerating innovation with T-Mobile ORG for business. Take your business further at T-Mobile.com ORG slash now. Support for Hidden Brain comes from BetterHelp Online Therapy ORG.We don't always realize just how much our negative thoughts and experiences stick with us. You may find your brain constantly running through a highlight read of bad moments, that hurtful comment your friend made, that frustrating thing your mom does, or that silly thing you said in a meeting. Maybe it's time to get it all off your chest. Therapy is a safe space to share whatever's weighing you down so you can get some relief and find a solution.BetterHelp ORG offers professional, affordable online therapy on a flexible schedule. Start the process in minutes and switch therapists anytime. Let it out with BetterHelp ORG. Visit BetterHelp.com slash hidden today to get 10% off your first month.

Speaker 91318.78s - 1328.68s

That's BetterHelp, H-E-L-P.com slash hidden. This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam PERSON.

Speaker 21329.26s - 1333.94s

When Vanessa Barnes was a graduate student at Columbia University ORG, she worked on a study.

Speaker 91334.74s - 1338.3s

Every day, she would leave her apartment in the Morningside Heights LOC neighborhood

Speaker 21338.3s - 1342.4s

and take the subway from 116th Street to Penn Station FAC.

Speaker 91344.98s - 1345.04s

Once she was there, she had to do something she found very difficult. from 116th Street to Penn Station FAC.

Speaker 21348.78s - 1349.36s

Once she was there, she had to do something she found very difficult.

Speaker 171352.22s - 1354.62s

Basically, I would just go up to random people in Penn Station FAC, say, hey, will you fill out this questionnaire?

Speaker 21357.32s - 1362.6s

Vanessa PERSON no longer remembers what the questionnaire was about, but she can still recall what it felt like to make such requests of total strangers.

Speaker 171365.84s - 1383.84s

Yeah, I mean, I still have flashbacks of going down to Penn Station FAC because it was so distressing. I would walk in. There'd be people kind of walking all over the place, and then there'd be people just sitting down waiting for their trains. So I'd usually go up to the person who was sitting there waiting for their train, you know, doing whatever they'd do to kind of occupy their time.And I would say, excuse me, will you please fill out the survey?

Speaker 21384.6s - 1403s

It felt incredibly awkward, stepping into someone's pace, disturbing them, asking them to stop doing what they were doing, and to do something she wanted them to do. As Vanessa PERSON asked for help and waited for an answer, her palms began to sweat. Her heart started beating faster.

Speaker 171403.28s - 1412.04s

It was a really sort of palpable fear that they were going to reject me or worse, right? Say something mean. I don't even know what, but I expected them to say something terrible.

Speaker 21413.26s - 1421.14s

Looking back on the moment now, it reminds her of another Stanley Milgram PERSON study, one that's less famous than the obedience experiment.

Speaker 171421.52s - 1427.58s

He had his research assistants, go on to New York City Subways FAC, and ask people for their seat.

Speaker 21427.94s - 1430.34s

Many of his students couldn't complete the task.

Speaker 171430.6s - 1443s

His students started coming back to him saying, I can't do this. This is just so upsetting. This is the most distressing thing you've ever asked me to do. And he was like, you guys are being babies. I don't understand why this is so upsetting.

Speaker 21443.9s - 1455.72s

And so, to prove his students wrong, the famous researcher set out for the subway himself. He would do what his students couldn't, walk up to strangers, and ask them for their seats.

Speaker 171456.22s - 1463.2s

He found the experience so much more distressing than he expected it to be, and all of a sudden he understood why they had been complaining so much.

Speaker 21464.24s - 1491s

Why is it so hard to make such requests? Well, one obvious explanation is that we know that people will reject us, and that rejection is painful. Vanessa PERSON remembers being hugely relieved when she was done giving out questionnaires at Penn Station FAC and could head back to her lab at Columbia University. Once there, she and her professor, Frank Flynn PERSON, analyzed the responses to the questionnaire. They noticed something intriguing.

Speaker 171491.42s - 1494.48s

Frank PERSON was like, I can't believe how many people are actually saying yes to you.

Speaker 21495s - 1506.02s

Total strangers, disrupted from reading their newspaper or eating a sandwich or watching the crowds of people in the busy station, they were like, sure, I'll respond to your questionnaire.

Speaker 171506.94s - 1511.86s

We were really surprised by how many people were agreeing in New York Penn Station ORG to do this survey.

Speaker 21512.62s - 1574.08s

What began as a simple observation turned into something much more important, an insight about our minds. Here's the chain of thought that led to the discovery. The reason Frank and Vanessa PERSON was surprised that so many people said yes is because they expected people to say no. If lots of people said yes, that meant that Vanessa PERSON's fears about rejection were misplaced. Her perception of the influence she actually had on other people was wrong. Like most of us, Vanessa PERSON had long felt that others had a big effect on her.As she gazed at the data, she realized that she had a big effect on other people. If she was blind to this power, what consequences could it have on her behavior? As researchers, the first thing that Vanessa and Frank PERSON decided to do was test if their personal experience was generalizable.

Speaker 171574.66s - 1601.08s

We decided to bring participants into the lab and have them do basically what I had done on those number of days. So we brought them into the lab. We said, hey, we're going to have you go out and ask people to, as our first step, fill out a survey, just like I had done. And how many people do you think are going to say yes to you? We made them estimate how many people they thought would agree, go out and actually ask people.And what we found was that they really underestimated the number of people who had agreed to that request.

Speaker 21601.62s - 1653.06s

So it wasn't just Vanessa and Frank PERSON. People in general seem to have a poor assessment of their power over others. People thought that others would find it easy to turn down their requests. Vanessa connected the seeming blind spot in our thinking to Stanley Milgram's PERSON famous obedience study. She realized this might be why everyone always saw the experiment from the perspective of thevolunteers asked to administer sharks, the people being influenced. No one saw the experiment from the point of view of the experimenter, the person exercising influence. We don't ask, was it hard for him to issue those crazy instructions because we don't identify with people exercising such influence? We think that kind of person must be very different from us because we don't identify with people exercising such influence. We think that kind of person must be very different from us because we don't feel we have such power. Was he surprised

Speaker 171653.06s - 1657.88s

to see these people going along with this crazy request he was making of them? So it's interesting

Speaker 21657.88s - 1675.52s

when people think about the Stanley Milgram PERSON study, and I think this is true for myself as well, I always imagine myself being in the role of the volunteer in the experiment, hearing the instructions from, you know, the experimenter saying, you know, you must shock this other person. I never put myself in the shoes of the experimenter. Exactly.

Speaker 171675.76s - 1692.92s

So that was something that we started to wonder about. So we so often sort of simulate, if I was in that Milgram Shock experiment, what would I do if I was the study participant, right? Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no? But we kind of flipped that idea on its head.

Speaker 21693.9s - 1721.5s

Vanessa went back to her experience at Penn Station FAC. It felt difficult because she had seen the interaction only from the point of view of her own insecurities. She hadn't seen the encounters through the point of view of the own insecurities. She hadn't seen the encounters through the point of view of the people she was asking for help. From their perspective, an anxious young woman was asking for something trivial.They had to weigh whether to put aside what they were doing and help her for a few minutes. If they said no, it could make them look like jerks.

Speaker 171721.78s - 1746.02s

It's this really interesting phenomenon where you have these two people interacting with one another, and they're both so focused on their own personal anxieties and insecurities and concerns with embarrassment that they don't realize that the other person is feeling that way, too. So it's this really interesting situation where being so inwardly focused on your own anxieties makes it so difficult for you to recognize what the situation really is for itself.

Speaker 21746.44s - 1799.7s

People in these encounters experience what psychologists call an egocentric bias. They are so consumed with their own perceptions that they fail to see what the interaction feels like for the other person. It's absolutely true that many of us are influenced by situations, that many of us will do things because the situation prompts it. But there is another problem too, and it might be a deeper problem.The people who put us in those situations, it's not like they are all-powerful gods. They are humans just like us, and they may not realize the extent of the power they have over us. In fact, they may be thinking, I'm sure this person is going to turn down my request.They might assume, falsely, that it's easy to refuse instructions. Vanessa PERSON realized that this bias could have all sorts of important consequences.

Speaker 171800.34s - 1824.1s

So what we started looking at about over a decade now ago, we started to look at whether we recognize when we're the ones who are influencing someone else. When we recognize that someone else, for example, can't say no to something that we've asked them. Vanessa has now a psychologist at Cornell University ORG.

Speaker 21824.74s - 1837.54s

In a series of experiments, she has demonstrated how people are often oblivious to the power that they have over others. In one study, she asked volunteers, mostly college students, to make a simple request of others.

Speaker 171837.84s - 1843.64s

We brought people into the lab, and we told them you're going to go out into campus and ask people to borrow their phones.

Speaker 21844.14s - 1851.22s

She walked them through how to approach someone and gave them instructions for what to do once people agreed to let them use their phones.

Speaker 171851.56s - 1858.98s

They would call us back at the lab and say, I have this person's phone, this is where I'm located, we'd mark it down, and then they go on and ask somebody else.

Speaker 21859.34s - 1868.5s

Before the volunteers went out to begin the study, Vanessa PERSON asked them a question. How many people would they have to ask to get three people to say yes?

Speaker 171868.92s - 1887.66s

And at this time, participants are kind of freaked out by this whole thought. They are convinced everyone's going to say no. They're not going to be able to do the task. And before they actually go out onto campus to do the task, they often would ask us, well, what if no one agrees? You know, do I come back? What do I do? They have all these concerns about, you know, not being able to complete the task.

Speaker 21891.16s - 1895.7s

What Vanessa found was similar to her own experience at Penn Station FAC. Many more people said yes than the volunteers expected.

Speaker 171897.46s - 1904.54s

They thought they had to ask a little over 10. They actually had to ask more like six. In fact, every other person was agreeing to this request.

Speaker 21905.26s - 1924.12s

Maybe you think the students had a high success rate because they were requesting something trivial. But Vanessa PERSON has also conducted a version of the study where volunteers had to ask for something more consequential. Money. For that study, she enlisted the help of the Leukemia and Lymphoma Society's Team in Training ORG program.

Speaker 171924.12s - 1941.3s

What people do when they participate in a fundraising activity for team and training is they ask people for donations so that they can participate in some sort of race, like a triathlon or a marathon. They get some training and some travel money to be able to do that, and the rest of the money actually goes to the organization.

Speaker 21941.9s - 1953.16s

Vanessa PERSON asked participants how many people they would have to solicit to meet their fundraising goals, which were typically thousands of dollars. They estimated they would need to ask about 200 people to meet the goal.

Speaker 171953.4s - 1960.22s

What we found is that they actually only had to ask about half that. So they only had to ask about 100 people in order to reach their fundraising goals.

Speaker 21960.68s - 1976.22s

Just as in Vanessa PERSON's phone study, her participants doubled the number of people they thought they had to ask to reach their goal. Their egocentric bias caused them to focus so much on their own anxieties that they ignored the influence they actually had over other people.

Speaker 171976.6s - 2008.62s

You're thinking about what you're asking. I'm asking this person for money. Will this person give me money? What you're not doing is thinking about what if you were sitting there, you know, potentially in your cubicle and a coworker came up to you and said, hey, I'm participating in a race. Would you be willing to sponsor me? If you were sitting there, it'd bereally hard to say no to your coworker, right? It'd be really hard to let them down. It'd be really awkward. What would you even say? And so people are kind of put on the spot and they find it really difficult to say no. So they go ahead and agree.

Speaker 22013.24s - 2039.46s

At the University of Chicago, economist John List PERSON has also studied the relationship between social pressure and charitable giving. John ran a study where experimenters knocked on the doors of some 8,000 houses in the Chicago area. They were trying to raise money for a children's hospital. John PERSON asked me to imagine the scenario from the point of view of the person receiving the request. Let's say it's a Sunday afternoon.

Speaker 182040.26s - 2042.28s

I've just made myself something to eat.

Speaker 152042.88s - 2043.52s

I'm relaxed.

Speaker 22043.52s - 2046s

You're sitting on the couch watching a football game,

Speaker 182046.26s - 2048.36s

and you hear somebody knocking on the door,

Speaker 22049.4s - 2051.48s

and you think, okay, should I get up,

Speaker 182051.58s - 2060.44s

or should I stay watching the football games? Of course, a lot of people get up and answer the door. But once they see that there's a solicitor at the door,

Speaker 92060.44s - 2062.52s

they say, oh my God,

Speaker 182062.88s - 2079.74s

I wish I would have stayed on the couch watching the football game. Too late. If they tell the solicitor no, then they have this very negative or disutility from letting someone down. So they're weighing that off versus just giving them $20 and having

Speaker 22079.74s - 2095.66s

them go on their way. John PERSON added a very interesting twist to the study. Some households were told ahead of time that a fundraising volunteer would come and knock on the door. Others were not told ahead of time. They just received an unexpected knock.

Speaker 182096.06s - 2118.64s

What we find is that when we warn them, of course, many people just stay on the couch or they leave the house. They never answer the door. The people who do answer the door, they do tend to give money. And much of that is because of altruistic reasons. But the people who we do not warn, they end up answering the door more often and they give more.

Speaker 22119.76s - 2140.9s

Put another way, people understand how they are going to feel when they're put on the spot. They often will go to great lengths to avoid getting in such situations. What this also means is that some significant portion of the money that charities raise might not come from altruism. In the case of the Children's Hospital ORG fundraiser, for example,

Speaker 182140.9s - 2160.52s

what you find is that roughly three quarters of the dollars given are due to social pressure, and a quarter of the dollars given is actually due to altruism. So a very small component of what we observe in our door-to-door fundraising drive is actually driven by altruism.

Speaker 22161.92s - 2177.7s

John's research reminds Vanessa of a classic study where researchers set up two boots on a college campus. One booth was clearly asking people for something, while the other did not ask for anything. What the researchers found was similar to John PERSON's donation study.

Speaker 172178.1s - 2193.6s

They measured how far away people walked from the booth as they walked by this path. And if people knew that they were going to be asked for something, their distance from the booth was much further than if if people knew that they were going to be asked for something, their distance from the booth was much further than if they didn't think they were going to be asked for something. We just kind of avoid any chance of having to say no to somebody.

Speaker 22197.36s - 2209.78s

We've seen how egocentric bias can cause us to act in helpful ways to others. We lend phones to people who need them or donate money to charity. Unfortunately, though, there's another side to the story.

Speaker 162210.38s - 2216.68s

They grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room. When we come back,

Speaker 22217.04s - 2242.42s

the sinister side of our inability to recognize our power over other people. You're listening to Hidden Brain WORK_OF_ART. I'm Shankar Vedant PERSON. Your business moves fast.

Speaker 112242.42s - 2299.9s

Like uploading a big presentation right before the meeting, processing multiple orders at the same time, trading files back and forth fast. So sort of fast internet just won't cut it. That's why more customers choose Comcast ORG business. And now we're introducing our fastest internet plans yet. It's ultimate speed for business. We're up to 12 times faster than Verizon,AT&T, and T-Mobile 5G business internet. Plus, existing Comcast ORG business customers could even get up to triple the speeds at no additional cost on our most popular plans. And it's all from the company with 99.9% network reliability and advanced cybersecurity. So you're not just getting super fast business internet. You're getting a network you can trust. It's ultimate speed for ultimate business. From Comcast Business ORG,powering possibilities. Compares Comcast Business ORG 1.25 gig to competitors 100 megabits per second 5G internet, three-time speed for essentials and standard internet customers.

Speaker 22300.9s - 2331.24s

Apple Card PRODUCT is the perfect cash-back rewards credit card. You earn up to 3% daily cash on every purchase every day. That's 3% on your favorite products at Apple ORG, 2% on all other Apple Card with Apple Pay PRODUCT purchases, and 1% on anything you buy with your titanium Apple Card PRODUCT or virtual card number. Visit apple.com slash card calculator to see how much you can earn.Apple card issued by Goldman Sachs Bank, USA, Salt Lake City GPE Branch, subject to credit approval. Terms apply.

Speaker 92336.44s - 2339s

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam PERSON.

Speaker 22339.8s - 2368.6s

When we interact with others, we are often intensely focused on how we feel. Our anxieties, our embarrassments, our fears. As a result, we're often blind to the effect we have on others, their anxieties, their embarrassments, and their fears. Psychologist Vanessa Bons PERSON has studied how such egocentric bias can keep people from asking for help. But that's not the whole story.

Speaker 172368.92s - 2422.28s

There's kind of the happy story, which is that people will help us more than we think. And then there's kind of the darker story, that people will do a lot of other things for us more so than we think. So we've run some studies where we started out kind of asking people if they could get someone to lie for them. So our original studies involved, you know, just filling something out. We said, what if we just have them ask if they'll, you know, sign their name to say, to something saying that you gave them a pitch that you didn't actually give them, just kind of a white lie.And so once again, we had people guess how many people they would have to ask this, make this request of before a certain number said yes. They went out onto campus. They asked people, you know, I'm supposed to be doing this pitch. I really don't feel like doing it. Well, you just sign this saying that I gave you the pitch. And again, most people wound up signing it, even though our participants thought that most people would say no.

Speaker 22422.92s - 2439.1s

As Vanessa PERSON says, the volunteers were asking people to tell a trivial lie. And perhaps you could say, what's the big deal in signing a note that says, someone gave you a pitch that they didn't? There are no real moral consequences. So Vanessa PERSON raised the stakes.

Speaker 172439.48s - 2491.14s

So what we did is we created these fake library books. We took a bunch of books off my bookshelf and just, you know, put some library codes on them. And we gave them to participants and we said, we're going to have you go into the libraries on campus and ask people to vandalize these library books. And so they were to tell people, I'm playing a prank of my friend, but they know my handwriting. Will you please just write pickle in this library book in pen?And they left it at that and looked at whether or not people agreed. And what we found is that the people they approached, so they kept track of sort of the things that people said when they made this request of them, and people would say things like, this is wrong, you shouldn't be doing this, we could get in trouble. They were clearly uncomfortable with the prospect of vandalizing this purported library book,but they still did it. They still did it.

Speaker 22491.78s - 2504.24s

And again, that finding went completely against the intuitions of the volunteers doing the asking. People significantly underestimated how much influence they possessed to get others to do something unethical.

Speaker 172505.18s - 2522.1s

So our participants, before they went out and started asking people, they thought about 28% of people would agree to do this, right? So they thought the vast majority of people would say no. But when they actually went out and made this request of people, 64%, a majority of the people they asked, actually agreed to vandalize this library book.

Speaker 22522.6s - 2529.28s

I mean, that's actually pretty astonishing that 64% of people would say yes. I mean, I would not have predicted it would be as high a number as that.

Speaker 172529.98s - 2550.12s

Yeah, I mean, this was a task we designed and we were like, this is never going to work, right? There's no way people are actually going to agree to do this. And we ourselves were completely surprised that people did agree. As much as it was uncomfortable for them to do this unethical thing and vandalize a library book, it was way more uncomfortable for them to say no to the person who was asking.

Speaker 22552.86s - 2584.2s

The scenarios in which egocentric bias could play a role in our behavior seem endless. We share the answers to our homework with a friend who asked to see our work. We don't push back when a colleague suggests bending the rules on a timesheet. We agree to keep a friend's infidelity a friend who asked to see our work. We don't push back when a colleague suggests bending the rules on a timesheet. We agree to keep a friend's infidelity a secret, even when it makes us uncomfortable. Listening to Vanessa PERSON made me realize why there is a vast gulf between what we predict we might do and what we actually do when we are confronted with problematic behavior.

Speaker 172584.2s - 2615.2s

We look as outsiders at the situation and we say, why would you tolerate this? Why wouldn't you just stand up and say, I'm not going to do this, I'm not going to hurt another person? I need to be able to do my job, and you're affecting my ability to do my job.But in fact, the social pressure, the concern about offending another person, the social anxiety in that situation is so palpable to that individual that it feels almost impossible for them to stand up and say something about it and reject this sort of behavior that they're encountering.

Speaker 22616.16s - 2635.24s

Hidden brain listener Anna Aburus PERSON called in with a story that illustrates how egocentric bias can affect workplace behavior. She was training to be an air traffic controller and saw examples of bullying and harassing behavior all around her. She says that trainers had a clear message for trainees.

Speaker 162635.24s - 2643.24s

Don't be soft or, you know, you got to have thick skin to survive in air traffic.

Speaker 22643.42s - 2644.78s

That was a common one for sure.

Speaker 162645.04s - 2649.64s

You have to have thick skin to survive in air traffic. That was a common one for sure. You have to have thick skin to survive in air traffic. I've heard that over a hundred times.

Speaker 22650.18s - 2652.26s

Anna recalled one painful incident.

Speaker 162652.26s - 2684.82s

There was a trainee that was trying to clear an aircraft for landing. And the trainer in that moment grabbed the headset of the trainee. And this headset is plugged in to the radar. And they grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room, which would fly off of the head of the trainee. They would actually tell them,hey, hurry up and go grab it so that you can plug back in and clear this aircraft for landing.

Speaker 22686.88s - 2699.12s

Anna PERSON says seeing such incidents made her fearful. She didn't feel she could complain since such behaviors appear to be the norm. Who could she complain to? The people who were themselves acting badly?

Speaker 162699.84s - 2706.14s

They would just say things like, what the fuck are you doing? If you do that again, I swear to God.

Speaker 22706.6s - 2725.78s

One time when she was directing aircraft, this was in real life, not a simulation, she found herself sitting next to one of the trainers, who she says had acted abusively toward trainees. By this point, Anna PERSON was no longer a trainee. She was directing two aircraft. One was 1,000 feet above the other.

Speaker 162726.02s - 2745.4s

I got the names of the aircraft mixed up. And I, because I was so nervous, I think, and I descended the wrong aircraft. I descended the one on topinstead of the one on the bottom. Because I got those call signs messed up.

Speaker 22746.12s - 2753.68s

She told the aircraft that was at a higher altitude to descend directly into the path of the lower altitude aircraft.

Speaker 162754s - 2808.46s

Luckily, the pilot could see the aircraft, so the pilot just said, no, we're not going to descend. And I immediately knew what I had just done. And I thought today was a clear day. It was clear skies. There was no clouds in the way.There wasn't any storm clouds in the way. But had there been storm clouds, or had there been some other kind of visual obstruction, this plane would have descended. And they would have hit that aircraft. And I would have been responsible forhundreds of deaths. And it wasn't because I didn't know how to control the traffic. I did. And I had done this a million times. It was because of the social stress that I was in at that time that didn't make me think clearly. I told Vanessa Barnes what Anna PERSON described,

Speaker 22808.84s - 2836.66s

how the mere presence of the trainer had disrupted her to the point where she made a mistake that could have been catastrophic. Vanessa PERSON said, look, it's certainly the case that there are lots of unethical people who know they are unethical and lots of bullies who know they are bullies. Maybe that was the case here.But there is a deeper problem in the workplace that we often forget, the bullies and harassers who don't know that they are bullies and harassers.

Speaker 172837.32s - 2864.22s

Often, when we're the person causing someone else distress, we can't see that distress. It's invisible to us. And it's not to let anybody off the hook because clearly it's the people creating this toxic culture's responsibility to kind of fix it and to not cause these things to happen. But there's also this cognitive bias there where we may not realize the extent to which we're interfering with somebody else's performance.

Speaker 22864.92s - 2870.26s

These same dynamics play out in another common occurrence in the workplace, unwanted romantic

Speaker 172870.26s - 2935.02s

attention. We ran a couple of studies where we asked people about their experiences being asked out at work or asking someone out at work. And we asked people to imagine situations where they weren't interested in the other person or the other person wasn't interested in them. And what we found is that people who asked somebody out at work and were rejected thought that it was pretty easy for that person to reject them, right? They didn't think that that person experienced a whole lot of distress, and they didn't think that they changed their behavior very much after being asked out.and they didn't think that they changed their behavior very much after being asked out. But when people recalled situations where they were asked out by someone at work who they weren't interested in, they described feeling obligated to say yes, feeling much more uncomfortable saying no to the person, and they reported doing all sorts of things to try to avoid that person that the other person didn't realize that they were doing. So in fact, this little request, you know, we tell people to just go for it and ask this person out, it actually puts a lot more pressure on the other person than we tend to realize when we're the ones doing the asking.

Speaker 22938.24s - 2941.54s

In some ways, we underestimate the pressure that we exert on other people. In some ways, that's the moral of the whole story here, isn't it?

Speaker 172942.44s - 2956.82s

Absolutely, yeah. We underestimate the influence that we have over other people, and we underestimate the extent to which asking them for something really puts them in an awkward position, because now they have to say no, and that's just a really hard thing for people to do.

Speaker 22958.24s - 2986.68s

Like many psychological biases, the tendency we have to downplay the influence we have on others can have far-reaching consequences. It can keep us from asking for help that would be forthcoming. It can keep us from reaching out and making friends with strangers. And it can also lead us to give into unethical demands or make improper demands of other people.I asked Vanessa PERSON how her research had prompted her to do things differently in her own life.

Speaker 172986.68s - 3021.36s

It has made a huge difference in the little things. So, for example, when I was pregnant, if I needed a seat on the subway or on a train, I would kind of stand there and look around and try to look my most pathetic so that someone would give me their seat, thinking that someone would step up and do it because they were nice, right? But in fact, everyone's all involved in their own stuff.They're not necessarily looking around and paying attention. And maybe they'd be perfectly happy to give up their seat, but they're not going to think of it unless you actually ask. And so I tried to take that into account.

Speaker 23021.36s - 3046.48s

So when I was pregnant, I would go up to people and be like, hey, can I sit down? I'd really love to sit. And then, of course, people are incredibly happy to just pop up and say yes. And what's interesting, of course, is when that happens, you're actually giving people an opportunity to do something nice. It's not just that you're imposing on them. Presumably, some of them are actually happy to say, you know, I was just writing to work and now I actually got to do this nice thing for this other person. I feel this little warm glow. Yeah, absolutely.

Speaker 173046.62s - 3077.58s

So a lot of people wonder about the takeaway. So if people agree to help us out of obligation because they feel like they can't say no, then do you really want to ask them for things? But people are really good at justifying their behaviors and ways that make them feel good about themselves. So they may agree to help because they feel like they can't say no, but pretty quickly after that, they're going to be convincing themselvesthat they helped because they're a really wonderful person. And so everyone's going to walk away, feeling good about the interaction. You got the help that you needed, and the other person gets to feel like a good person.

Speaker 23081.94s - 3212.56s

Psychologists once conducted a lighthearted version of Stanley Milgram's PERSON obedience study. In the 1970s, they had research assistants stand on the streets of New York City GPE. Their jobs? To look skyward? At nothing. The question the researchers wanted to know was whether innocent passers-by would also stop to look upto see what was going on. They found that when more people were in on the gag, more pedestrians stopped and looked up. I've seen video of that study many times and always found this scene funny, one to 15 people just staring off into the sky. Recently, I re-watched it, and this time I did what Vanessa PERSON had done. I flipped the script.Instead of seeing the experiment from the point of view of the passers-by and asking myself whether I would be similarly influenced, I looked at the experiment from the point of view of the research assistants. Did they expect so many people to join them in looking at nothing? We've seen throughout this episode how all of us as individuals have great power to shape how others behave. If each of us has this hidden power, then collectively as groups, as communities, as tribes, we are going to have even more influence.How we choose to use that influence? That's up to each of us. Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes Annie Murphy Paul PERSON, Kristen Wong, Laura Querell, Ryan Katz PERSON, Autumn Barnes, Andrew Chadwick, and Nick Woodbury PERSON. Tara Boyle PERSON is our executive producer.I'm Hidden Brain's ORG executive editor. We end today with a story from our sister show, My Unsunk Hero WORK_OF_ART. It's brought to you by T-Mobile for Business ORG. Our story comes from Bethany Renfrey PERSON.

Speaker 153213.18s - 3363.58s

I was 20 years old. My baby girls and I were living in low-income apartments. Most of my neighbors were single mothers like myself. And I remember how overwhelmed I felt that morning. It was a cold day in the apartment. I dragged myself to the sink and it was stacked with dishes with pots and pans that had been soaking because I'd burnt them all. I didn't know how to cook back then and I would always burn ourpans. And I looked at my twin girls. They were 18 months old. They sat in their high chairs. The baby, the newborn was in her swing. I looked back at the sink and I just couldn't bring myself to do those dishes and I couldn't look at them any longer. It was a reminder of how overwhelmed I felt in my own life. So I grabbed a white garbage bag and I stacked the dishes in there one by one. I walked out in the rain and I placed it on the edge of the apartment dumpster because the dumpster was full. And I came back in and the girls and I left for the day. When I got back that evening, it was dark and my porch was dark because I didn't even have the energy to change the porch light. But as we were coming in, I kind of kicked something. It was a box.And so I brought it into the apartment and put it on the table. And it was my pots and pans. And they were shining and sparkling. And the girls' blues, clues clues plates and their sippy cups and a little handwritten note popped out on a yellow piece of paper and it said,I've been there before, you will make it, I promise you. I don't know which of the single mothers went out there that day and saw that garbage bag and understood what was happening. But if I saw her today, I would thank her for showing me that we are not alone and we are not bad mothers, even in our hardest moments. We are surrounded by kindness and understanding. And I'm so grateful to have learned that lesson so early on in motherhood.Bethany Renfrey lives in Sutter Creek, California.

Speaker 23364.08s - 3420.14s

When she was 27, she went back to school and earned undergraduate and master's degrees. She's now a legislative director in the California State Senate. Today's My Unsung Hero story was brought to you by T-Mobile for Business ORG. If you enjoy Hidden Brain, please take a moment and share your favorite episode with two or three people in your life. Word of mouth recommendations really make a huge difference in introducing new listeners to our work. We truly appreciate your support.I'm Shankar Vedantham PERSON. See you soon.

Speaker 93423.56s - 3425.1s

Your space coast vacation is preparing for a liftoff. Start counting down now.

Speaker 143425.54s - 3449.4s

10, 9, 8, 7, it's time for a beach vacay that feels like heaven. 6, 5, 4, come explore Melbourne and the beaches. 3, 2, 1. It's time for some rocket-filled fun. Count down to your best beach vacation ever on Florida GPE's space coast. Launch your planning now at visit on Florida's Space Coast LOC. Launch your planning now at visitspacost.com.

Speaker 123451.8s - 3476.38s

Just in and so good. Thousands of summer deals at your Nordstrom Rack ORG store. Save up to 60% on new arrivals from Vince ORG, rag and bone, Adidas, Joe's, Mark Jacobs ORG, and more. Great brands, great prices every day at Nordstrom Rack ORG. But hurry for first dibs.Get your summer favorites up to 60% off at Nordstrom Rack today. Great brands, great prices. That's why you rack.

Speaker 133479.58s - 3539.32s

Why are smart businesses graduating to NetSuite by Oracle ORG? Because NetSuite ORG eliminates the expense of multiple business systems by consolidating your operations together into one. NetSuite is the number one cloud financial system, bringing accounting, financial management, inventory, and HR into one platform and one source of truth. NetSuite ORG reduces IT costs because it lives in the cloud with no hardware required so you can access it from anywhere. You cut the cost and headaches of maintaining multiple systems because you've cut one unified business management suite. Bringing all your major business processes into one platform improves efficiency, slashing manual tasks and errors. Over 37,000 companies havealready made the move, so do the math. You'll see how you'll profit with NetSuite ORG too. And now by popular demand, NetSuite has extended its one-of-a-kind flexible financing program for a few more weeks. Just go to NetSuite.com slash Podcast 25 for more information. That's netsuite.com slash podcast 25. Shopify ORG is the global commerce platform that helps you sell at

Speaker 13539.32s - 3565.08s

every stage of your business. With the Internet's best converting checkout, 36% better on average compared to other leading commerce platforms. Shopify ORG helps you turn browsers into buyers. In fact, Shopify powers 10% of all e-commerce in the U.S. Sign up for a $1 per month trial period at Shopify.com ORG slash podcast free. All lowercase, Shopify.com ORG slash podcast free.Shopify.com ORG slash podcast free.